|
Εγγραφή | Συχνές Ερωτήσεις | Social Groups | Ημερολόγιο | Αναζήτηση | Σημερινά Μηνύματα | Σημειώστε όλα τα forums ως διαβασμένα |
Bettas Μονομάχοι, όλων των μορφών. |
|
Εργαλεία Θεμάτων | Τρόποι εμφάνισης |
#1
|
||||
|
||||
![]()
BETTAS...AND MORE
By Dr. Leo Buss Genetics and Show Classification On the show Betta circuit, it is a maxim that the standards by which fish are judged are not isomorphic with our understanding of Betta genetics. Some classes are founded on single-gene effects on pigment (e.g., black), others distribute the effects of single-gene into multiple classes (e.g., steel, blue, turquoise), and yet other show classes deliberately combine colors known to be genetically distinct (e.g., yellow and clear). Moreover, some pigmentation genes have no show class(es) predicated upon their effects (e.g., black lace). The classic example of the mismatch between genetics and show categorization is the fact that a fish carrying the mutant allele of the opaque gene can be shown in either the opaque class or the pastel class, depending upon whether the opaque effect is, or is not, evident. At first blush this may appear an untidy and unfortunate way to organize affairs. There are, however, a number of quite good reasons why one would not like to arrange matters on strictly genetic lines. As a simple practical matter, if we were to create a class for every genetic combination of even the well-known genes, we would have vastly too many classes to deal with. Consider the numbers. With one gene, we will have either two or three recognizably different forms, corresponding to genes that are dominant or codominant respectively. So that we are dealing with minimal numbers in the discussion to follow, let us imagine that genes yield only two alternative appearances. If we designate a gene by a number and its distinct appearance by a letter, then a single gene gives two combinations (1A is gene 1 producing appearance A, 1B is gene 1 producing appearance B). Now, consider a second gene. We have four possible combinations (1A2A, 1A2B, 1B2A, 1B2B). A third gene ramifies the possibilities: 1A2A3A, 1A2A3B, 1A2B3A, 1A2B3B, 1B2A3A, 1B2A3B, 1B2B3A, and 1B2B3B. The effects of a single gene gives 2 possibilities, two genes 4 possibilities, and three genes 8 possibilities. The series of 1,2, 3 genes yields 2, 4, 8 possible combinations, so the general solution is the number of combinations equals 2m, where m=the number of genes. Thus if we were to organize show classes on genetically separable criterion, 10 genes would yield 1,024 different show classes. Inevitably only a handful of very specific combinations map to existing show classes. These particular combinations are chosen not on genetic grounds per se, but because these specific combinations of genes yield a pleasing result. Humans like pure colors and contrasting patterns. The show classes reflect these aesthetics. The tiny subset of vast universe of possible genetic combinations that do correspond to show categories are chosen because they generate the purest solid colors or those that produce the most striking of patterns. For example, the show class for blacks calls for the fish to be black, only black, and the blackest of imaginable black. The ideal is never realized, but is best approximated when the show animal has two copies of the mutant allele at the melano locus (=gene). This genetic configuration is necessary, but not sufficient. We further require that the animal have one or no copies of the mutant allele at the cambodia locus, that the animal have two copies of the wild-type allele at the "spread iridescence" locus, as well as various other preferred states at each of the other known genes. Aesthetic criteria define the ends; genetics provide the means. No less compelling a reason for the decoupling of show classes from the underlying genetics is that many eye-pleasing forms and color patterns of Betta do not have adequate genetic interpretations. We lack a satisfactory genetic understanding of most of the pattern variations. Pattern variations include bicolor, the appearance of one color on the fins and another on the body; butterfly, the appearance of two colors bisecting the fins along the proximal-distal axis; and marble, the ontogenetic appearance and disappearance of pigments in patches in the body and (to a lesser extent) fins. All await genetic explanation. A genetically-based show system would preclude such forms. A show system based on genetics would likewise impose intolerably long delays in introducing new show categories. New colors and new forms of Betta are continuously being developed. Among the colors, orange and copper are recent additions. Recent form variants include the crowntail, a striking and extreme form of the combtail trait, the halfmoon discussed in my column of November 2003, and the short moon, an attractive new form generated by repeated crosses of halfmoon fish to short-finned plakats. The orange color was developed by selective breeding by Gilbert Limhengco and is now being "worked" by a number of breeders. The copper color emerged from Thailand in 2002 and is said to have been developed by introgression from wild animals. Although anecdotes abound, hard data of the transmission of these traits in crosses between animals of known phenotype are lacking. There is no reason, though, that the show circuit must wait until someone has the inclination to make the half-dozen or so crosses required to establish the genetics issues firmly. Science takes time, commitment, and money. The show circuit cannot be held hostage to waiting for this constellation of conditions to be satisfied. While neither orange nor copper and its variants are yet recognized as classes in the International Betta Congress (IBC) show system, their inclusion is not precluded by a lack of genetic understanding. For the perspective of a breeder of show animals, the concern is not precise predictability, as guaranteed by the "dance of the chromosomes" which yields simple Mendelian ratios, but only that the trait appears with sufficient predictability to permit and sustain a breeding program. While genetics need not be a precise map to show categories, genetic considerations are far from irrelevant. Indeed, they are an essential component of the entire enterprise. The mission of the IBC includes the preservation and enhancement of the animal. The show classes put the power of our competitive spirit to meet the mission of enhancement. Setting standards at the highest level, even if those goals are rarely realized, guarantees that breeders will continue to attempt to produce the cleanest of reds, yellowest of yellows and the blackest of blacks. An understanding of the underlying genetics allows one to plot the most rapid route to this desired end. It allows one to appreciate that the quickest route, for example, to greens is not by breeding blue to yellow, but rather by breeding blue to blue. The show system plays another vital, albeit less appreciated role. Establishing and maintaining show categories attracts breeders to those categories. This, in turn, assures that the relevant genetic combinations specific to that category are preserved. This point bears elaboration and emphasis. If there is not a system in place that assures otherwise, genetic variation will be lost. The relevant genetics here are not the inheritance rules known to most breeders, but are rather derived from an allied field called population genetics. Population genetics, to a first approximation, can be thought of as the science that arises when one combines the Mendel's inheritance rules with demography. It deals with genetic expectations within whole populations. Among the problems posed to population geneticists are ones like: "How large must a population of endangered species be to ensure that genetic information will not be lost" or "How many seeds must be stored to ensure that we preserve the known genetic diversity of maize." These questions are but rephrasing of the one we wish to ask. Imagine that there were no show system and that breeders did not design crosses based on an appreciation of genetics. If all crosses were made by choosing parents at random, the odds of losing an allele by chance alone is approximated by 1/2N, where N is the number of breeders. If we assume that the number of active show breeders in the IBC in any given year is 15, then the chance of losing an allele in that year is 6.7%, an appreciable risk. Given that this risk would apply year in and year out, it is effectively certain that genetic variation would eventually be lost unless some convention precludes it. Population genetics tells us that genetic variation is vulnerable to extinction in a small population of breeders. It likewise tells us how this danger can be mitigated. There are two ways. We could breed at random, but increase the number of breeders. If we had 100 active breeders, the chance of losing an allele is vastly lower (0.1%). An alternative solution lies in non-random breeding. Indeed, this is precisely how the show system ameliorates extinction risk. By providing classes for specific combinations, it essentially guarantees that someone will breed that combination and in so doing guarantees that the genetic variation required to produce that combination is maintained. If we are concerned with maintaining the genetic variation required to produce an opaque Betta, that variation will be maintained as long as a single breeder is working opaques. The judging standards and show system acts as a modulator of the population genetics of Betta. Recognizing this is helpful. One might look at the fish at an annual convention and bemoan the fact that "the cambodians are not what they used to be" or that "there are no good pastels," and so on. Such variation in quality is inevitable with a large number of classes in a labor intensive hobby in a world where most find themselves time-constrained. Yet, if we remind ourselves that the foremost reason for a show category is preservation and that enhancement is a secondary consideration, today's poor cambodias are insurance that variation is available for future enterprising breeders. Indeed, a wise judge will take a new breeder on a tour of classes with less than stellar animals and explain what might be achieved. These considerations hardly exhaust the interface between judging standards and genetics. Any number of topics are germane. Is it more important to establish a class if the new trait is polygenic than if it is a simple Mendelian trait? [Yes] Do systems established on a color hierarchy result in larger number of classes than systems based on other criteria (e.g., form, pattern)? [Not necessarily] If classes ever need to be eliminated or combined, are there genetic grounds for preferring one elimination of one class over another. [Yes] The job of maintaining International Betta Congress (IBC) show standards and judging criterion falls to the IBC Judging Board (JB). The importance of their work is recognized by the special status accorded their decisions. While all other matters are subject to membership approval, the show standards are solely the responsibility of the JB. What makes the work of JB among the most challenging is not the technical content emanating from genetics, but rather the vastly more complex matter of balancing genetic knowledge with aesthetic sense in a fashion that supports rather than erodes the social fabric of the organization. This may in practice be quite difficult to achieve, but then that is what makes the job of interest.
__________________
Matthew. Vagionakis http://www.malawicichlidhomepage.com...agionakis.html http://www.malawicichlidhomepage.com...ma_loreto.html |
Συνδεδεμένοι χρήστες που διαβάζουν αυτό το θέμα: 1 (0 μέλη και 1 επισκέπτες) | |
Εργαλεία Θεμάτων | |
Τρόποι εμφάνισης | |
|
|